Grammar: Words and Their Arrangement
Form and Function of the English Clause
A sentence is a sound in itself on which other sounds called
words are strung.
|
Robert Frost, Selected Letters
|
The word
"grammar" has many meanings. For some people, grammar specifies the
"correct" way to speak or write. For others, the word refers to the
inflections (the word endings) common in many languages. For still others, the
grammar is about how humans organize ideas into words. The word
"grammar" means all of those things. But, for us, the word means
something quite specific: grammar describes how we choose and arrange our
words.
Yet
grammar is more than passively learning ideas about the organization of words
in a language. Grammar is also an activity; it is something we do. For example,
consider this paragraph from the opening of Anthony Burgess's A Clockwork
Orange.
There was me, that is Alex, and my three droogs, that is
Pete, Georgie, and Dim, Dim being really dim, and we sat in the Korova Milkbar
making up our rassoodocks what to do with the evening, a flip dark chill winter
bastard though dry. The Korova Milkbar was a milk-plus mesto, and you may, O my
brother, have forgotten what these mestos were like things changing so skorry
these days and everybody very quick to forget, newspaper not being read much
neither. Well, what they sold there was milk plus something else. They had no
license for selling liquor, but there was no law yet against prodding some of
the new veshches which they would put into the old moloko, so you could peet it
with vellocet or synthemesc or drencrom or one or two other veshches which
would give you a nice quiet horrorshow fifteen minutes admiring Bog And All His
Holy Angels And Saints in your left shoe with lights bursting all over your mozg.
Or you could peet it with knives in it, as we use to say, and this would
sharpen you up and make you ready for a bit of dirty twenty-to-one, and that
was what we were peeting this evening I'm starting off the story with.
As you
read the paragraph above, your thinking probably went through several stages:
first, you noticed the unusual words and felt a certain uneasiness about the
language; it seemed to be English, but not quite right. Then, you probably
noticed that several of the unusual words had ending that you recognized or
were surrounded by familiar words. Finally, having guessed at the meaning of
those unusual words from context clues in that paragraph, you could reread the
paragraph more easily. In other words, you were acting as a grammarian already:
you
- observed the language data (by noticing the unusual words in their contexts),
- collected a few pertinent facts (by noticing that several words were placed near function words like the or of and by noticing word endings like -s or -ing, clues to how the strange words functioned in those clauses),
- made and tested a hypothesis (by rereading a sentence after revising mentally to add the information you collected by noticing the word's position and endings), and
- reached a conclusion (that your hypothesis was correct because the paragraph made more sense).
In short,
those are the same steps any linguist takes when studying any phenomenon in
language, including grammar.
Furthermore,
understanding that paragraph from A Clockwork Orange means understanding
(at least at some level) and using all the fundamental concepts of grammar:
categories, constituency, and metafunctions. The concept of category allows us
to recognize that several of the unfamiliar words belong to the word category
'noun.' Another concept, constituency, allows us to recognize that several
unusual sequences of words in the first paragraph (such as Bog And All His
Holy Angels And Saints or Korova Milkbar) must be single units,
despite the fact that they are odd sequences of words. Finally, through a
concept of metafunction, we are able to recognize several additional facts
about that paragraph: even though the words are unusual for English, the
sentences are statements (rather than question or commands); moreover, the
'theme' of paragraph is largely about Alex's perceptions about milkbars,
particularly the Korova Milkbar.
Yet before
we begin to explore more examples of the grammar of human language at work,
let's first settle some initial concerns: what does 'grammar' mean and what is
the place of grammar in the structure of language as a whole?
SOME PRELIMINARIES
Grammar is
about how units of language are sequenced, since quite obviously language
proceeds sequentially, linearly: in speech, one sound is uttered before the
next, one syllable before the next, one word before the next, and so on; in
writing, one word precedes the next, one phrase precedes the next, one clause
precedes the next, and so on. So at some point in the production (and the same
is true in the inverse for the perception) of language, humans must take all
their thoughts, requests, desires, and hopes that are relevant within a
particular context of situation and produce language that expresses those
meanings and organizes those ideas sequentially. The same is true in the
inverse for the perception of language.
Now one
might quite rightly ask doesn't the word 'grammar' alone suggest some
sequential arrangement of linguistic units? Yes, the idea of grammar (both in
its popular and in several of its technical senses) does emphasize the
importance of the right sequence of words, phrases, and clauses within a
sentence. However, there are many reasons why those of us who are fascinated by
language and interested in accurately describing and explaining how language
works should pay close attention to words in grammar. For our purposes here, we
will discuss only two.
First, if
we were to ask people what they thought were the fundamental building blocks of
language, they would very likely say "Words" more than any other
response (with "syllable" the only real competition). Words seem to
be the most obvious component of language, and any theory that fails to account
for the contribution of words to the functioning of language is unworthy of our
attention. (See Halliday 1994.) So for that reason alone we need to include
words in our study of grammar. Moreover, there are many other, less obvious,
reasons why we need to attend to words in our grammatical description.
To
illustrate this second reason for the importance of words in our description of
grammar, consider sentences (1) through (4):
- The water evaporated.
- The dog evaporated.
- The water evaporated quickly.
- The water evaporated the dog.
Sentences
(1) through (4) illustrate that the word evaporate is restricted in its
usage in quite specific ways. But if we had only to work with a grammar of
English that examines grammatical structures without referring to the lexicon,
we would quickly discover the weakness of our grammatical analysis. For
example, looking at the structure of the sentences in (1) through (4), we see
some really quite ordinary arrangements of clause elements, arrangements that
occur regularly in English. In (1) and (2), we have the Subject-Verb (SV)
pattern; in (3) we see the Subject-Verb-Adverbial (SVA) pattern; in (4) we find
the Subject-Verb-Object (SVO) pattern. Now a grammar that ignores the lexicon
will describe all the sentences in (1) through (4) as well-formed structures
(since they are after all very common clause patterns), only to realize the
inadequacy of such a description and propose some kind of remedy to the problem
elsewhere in the theory.
However,
if the theory incorporates into its grammar all the meaningful distinctions
found in the lexicon, then there need be no division between the grammatical
and lexical 'levels' in our analysis of language. In our example, in other
words, our grammar will be sensitive to the fact that in real language, we must
distinguish between transitive (verbs that can occur with an object in an SVO
pattern) and intransitive (verbs that may not occur with an object, but may
occur with some other complement, such as an adverbial, as in the SV and SVA
patterns) early on in our description of the language. Thus, by recognizing
that some forms function as intransitive verbs, we can explain why (4) seems so
peculiar, while (1) and (3) seem quite ordinary. Further, if our description of
the language — our grammar that is — also recognizes the distinction between
verbs that can co-occur with 'agent' subjects as opposed to those verbs that do
not, then we can explain why (2) seems odd.
Now that
we have some sense of why the lexicon contributes so substantially to our
understanding of a language's grammar, we may go on to consider grammar's place
in the structure of language. Halliday posits four strata at work,
simultaneously, in the production and perception of language:
- the context of the language situation
- meaning (semantics)
- wording (grammar)
- sound patterns (phonology and phonetics)
To
illustrate how the different levels of language co-exist, consider the odd
examples of sentences (2) and (4) again. Given our conventional world as the
context for the language situation we are in at this moment, sentences like (2)
and (4) do seem strange since they suggest ideas that we recognize as unlikely.
But, if we changed the context of situation to that of science fiction,
suddenly sentences like (2) and (4) make sense. Assuming some alternate
universe where our laws of physics are pushed beyond our experience or
altogether void, sentences (2) and (4) are meaningful.
For
another example, consider these sets of contexts and sentences: all four
sentences are requests for an open window from the speaker to a listener.
However, the context of situation is different for each sentence. Which
sentence goes with which situation below?
Sentence
|
Situation
|
||
(a)
|
Pardon me, sir, but would you mind
opening the window?
|
(i)
|
Doing homework with your girl-/boyfriend.
|
(b)
|
Open the window, will ya buddy?
|
(ii)
|
Baby-sitting your snotty younger brother.
|
(c)
|
Open the window NOW.
|
(iii)
|
Meeting with the dean in his office.
|
(d)
|
Gee, it's hot in here.
|
(iv)
|
Sitting on a bus next to a man dressed in
work clothes.
|
If you
found that you could match (a) with (iii), (b) with (iv), (c) with (ii), and
(d) with (i), then you were experiencing how context, meaning, and wording all
mutually interact in language.
In (a),
notice that the context of situation (meeting with the dean in his office)
places us in a socially less powerful position. After all, we are in his
office; we are not on his social/political level. Therefore, when we make a
request of someone 'above' us on some social scale, notice how the meaning and
the wording (grammar) change: the request is not in the form of a command, but
more politely put in the form of a question. Yet even though the grammar of (a)
is the form of a question that could be answered by yes or no, we
do not mean it to be interpreted as a question, nor do we expect it to be
interpreted by the dean as a yes/no question. (Indeed, if the dean
answered "No," we could most likely interpret the response as either
an attempt at humor or an act of hostility.) Other markers of politeness in (a)
are the speaker's request for permission to speak (Pardon me) and the
vocative (sir).
In (b) and
(c), we find two forms that are commands. We can distinguish them and match
their contexts because of the differing markers of politeness in each. In (b),
the tag question at the end of the command undercuts the forcefulness of the
command, as does the familiar term of address (buddy) and the informal
pronunciation of the pronoun you. Those features of grammar point to a
situation in which one is probably speaking to a stranger in a close situation.
In (c), however, one finds the same command ending with a heavily stressed time
adverbial, highlighting through the grammar the forcefulness of the command and
the speaker's social power.
In (d), we
have another form that is not a command at all grammatically: it is a
statement. But notice that it begins with a word to indicate the speaker's
discomfort (Gee), and notice that the word hot is ambiguous in
this context, possibly referring either to the speaker's temperature or the
speaker's excitement in this context.
In (a)
through (d), we can see, therefore, that the context of situation, the meaning,
the grammar, and even the sound patterns mutually interact to create the
language we use. But notice in those examples that the choices of wording
(grammar) do not simply encode meaning: rather, the grammar makes
meaning. That distinction is fundamental between this theory of language
(systemics) and other theories of grammar.
To
understand more about grammar, we must now examine the grammatical constituents
of language (categories), how those constituents combine at different levels
(constituency), and how those combinations of constituents can create meaning
(metafunction).
Form and Function of the Clause in English
Words and
phrases are the constituents of the clause rank. In order to discuss the
constituents of the clause, it is necessary to refer to the units smaller than
the clause itself. Consider the following example, in which we can see that a
single clause is composed of smaller units of the phrase rank.
From our
discussion of the phrase rank, we also know that we can categorize the
constituents of that clause into the appropriate phrase type.
Furthermore,
we also know that each phrase can be subcategorized into its constituent parts.
The
diagram above, however, looks at the constituents of the clause only from the
perspective of the constituents' forms. We should remember that those
forms also serve functions, just as the forms at the phrase rank can
also be described according to the functions they served within their phrases.
Essentially the clause can be divided into eight functional constituents,
grouped into five categories:
Functional Categories
|
Eight Clause Functions
|
(1) Subject
|
[1] Subject
|
(2) Verb
|
[2] Verb
|
(3) Objects
|
[3] Direct Object
|
[4] Indirect Object
|
|
(4) Complements
|
[5] Object Complement
|
[6] Subject Complement
|
|
(5) Adverbials
|
[7] Adverbial Complement
|
[8] Adverbial
|
At the
clause rank, the constituents marked in the example sentence above serve four
different clause functions: subject (S) as in The news, verb (V) as in has
been, subject complement (SC) as in quite sad, and adverbial (A) as
in in fact. We use the following abbreviations for the other four clause
functions: direct object (DO), indirect object (IO), object complement (OC),
and adverbial complement (AC). The examples below illustrates some other clause
patterns that are possible in English.
- Liz (S) is resting (V) quietly (A) in the other room (A).
- The mind (S) is (V) immensely complex (SC).
- The children (S) were (V) here (AC) all morning (A).
- Emily (S) is playing (V) cards (O) with her sister (A).
- Early next week (A), the President (S) will send (V) Congress (IO) his budget (DO).
- Clearly (A), the committee (S) considers (V) her (DO) the best (OC).
- Once again (A), I (S) will put (V) the book (DO) away (AC).
When we
look at the examples, we notice that each clause has a different arrangement of
functional elements, but there are some patterns too. First, we notice that
while the different clauses have different arrangements of objects,
complements, and adverbials, each clause consistently has a subject and verb.
Thus, in the declarative clause, we call the functions of subject and verb the
'central' functions while objects, complements, and adverbials are the
'peripheral' functions. We also notice that adverbials are 'optional' when
compared to the other clause constituents. That is, we could easily eliminate
all the adverbials in sentences (1) through (7) and still have a well-formed
English clause remaining. By eliminating the optional adverbials, then, we arrive
at a classification of the basic clause patterns on the basis of the
'obligatory' constituents.
Some Examples of the Seven Clause
Patterns in English
|
|||||||
S
|
V
|
IO
|
DO
|
SC
|
OC
|
AC
|
|
SV
|
Liz
|
is resting
|
|||||
SVC
|
The mind
|
is
|
complex
|
||||
SVA
|
The kids
|
were
|
here
|
||||
SVO
|
Emily
|
is playing
|
cards
|
||||
SVOO
|
will send
|
Congress
|
his budget
|
||||
SVOC
|
We
|
consider
|
her
|
the best
|
|||
SVOA
|
I
|
will put
|
the book
|
away
|
|||
This set
of patterns is the most general classification that can be usefully applied to
the English clause. Correlating with the seven clause patterns are the three
main types of verbs:
- intransitive verbs, followed by no obligatory constituents, as in SV pattern above;
- copular verbs, followed by a SC or AC, as in the SVC and SVA patterns above; and
- transitive verbs, followed by an object, as in the SVO, SVOO, SVOC, and SVOA patterns above.
To
conclude this outline of the basic clause patterns, we need to understand the
principles by which the functional constituents of the clause are identified.
Although the categories of S, V, DO, IO, OC ,
SC , AC, and A are functional
constituents, they are identifiable by both formal and functional criteria.
The Subject
The
subject, like the verb, is a central constituent in the clause. And as a
central element, it 'governs' many of the grammatical choices to be made within
the clause. The subject determines agreement between itself and the verb and
governs the person, number, case, and gender, where relevant, of several other
constituents within the clause. Traditional grammar books and school grammars
often define the subject along semantic lines: they refer to the subject as
"what the sentence is about" or as "the topic of the
sentence" or as the "actor performing the action described by the
verb. From the perspective of grammar, however, such definitions are
misleading, since those older definitions blend and conflate different ideas
that are best understood if kept apart. As we will see soon in the section on
METAFUNCTION, the subject is a distinct entity, related to, but separate from,
notions like 'theme' or 'topic' or 'actor.'
Grammatical
subjects usually share a number of properties that serve to identify them
within a clause. These are form, position, agreement, pronouns, and voice.
Form. The subject is usually
a noun phrase or clause.
That guy (S) is the one.
What I don't know (S) can
hurt me.
Position. The subject is
usually positioned before the verb in the indicative mood, after the auxiliary
in the interrogative mood, and absent — but implied — in the imperative mood.
Did I (S) win? [interrogative mood]
Go home! [imperative mood]
Agreement. Subjects usually
determine the number (singular or plural) and person (first, second, third) of
other constituents in the clause. The subject determines the number and person,
where relevant, of the verb in finite clauses.
Liz (S) works (V)
hard. [singular, third person]
Emily and Liz (S) work
(V) hard. [Plural]
The
subject determines the number and person, where relevant, of any noun phrase
functioning as the subject complement.
Frank (S) is my nephew
(SC)
Frank, Mike, and Paul (S)
are my nephews (SC)
The
subject also determines the number, person, and gender, where relevant, of
'reflexive' pronouns — pronouns ending with the -self or -selves
inflections.
I (S) cut myself.
They (S) cut themselves.
The
subject determines the number, person, and gender, where relevant, of the
emphatic pronoun own in structures such as my own or their own.
I (S) cut myself with my
own knife.
They (S) cut themselves with
their own knives.
Pronouns. The subject
determines the case, where relevant, of any pronoun in subject function; that
is, the subject function requires the subjective form of the pronoun.
I (S) like her.
She (S) likes me.
Voice. There is a systematic
relationship between the subject of a clause and voice (active or passive). The
active voice subject corresponds to an adverbial (beginning with the
preposition by) in the passive voice.
Emily (S) likes Liz. [active
voice]
Liz is liked by Emily (A) [passive voice].
The Verb
The
function of verb is the least ambiguous in English since only one category (the
verb phrase) fills this function. Nonetheless, the verb is recognizable by a
combination of formal and functional properties. These are form, position,
agreement, tense, modality, aspect, and voice.
Form. The verb is composed
of a main verb with or without auxiliaries.
Frank reads (V) quickly.
Frank must have been reading (V) quickly.
Position. The verb usually
occurs after the subject in the indicative mood, around the subject in the
interrogative mood, and at the beginning of the clause in the imperative mood.
Did I win (V)?
[interrogative mood]
Go (V) home! [imperative
mood]
Agreement. The verb
corresponds in number (singular or plural) with the subject in finite clauses.
Liz (S) works (V)
hard. [singular, third person]
Emily and Liz (S) work
(V) hard. [plural]
Tense. The verb of a finite
clause is marked for tense (present or past).
Mike likes (V) Nintendo. [present]
Mike liked (V) Nintendo. [past]
Modality. The verb can be
marked as predicating something other than simple fact (modal).
Paul might do (V) it. [modal]
Paul ought to do (V) it. [semi-modal]
Aspect. The verb can be
marked as completing or continuing the process indicated by the main verb
(perfect and/or progressive).
Paul has done (V) it. [perfect]
Paul is doing (V) it. [progressive]
Paul has been doing (V) it for a long time. [perfect
and progressive]
Voice. The verb is marked
for voice (active or passive).
Emily likes (V) Liz. [active voice]
Liz is liked (V) by Emily. [passive voice]
The Direct Object
The direct
object is identifiable by its formal and functional properties. These are form,
position, pronouns, and voice.
Form. The direct object
usually has the form of a noun phrase or clause.
The cat chased the mouse (DO).
I know that she will be here soon (DO).
Position. Direct objects
usually occur after the subject and verb, as in the examples above.
Pronouns. If the subject and
the object of a clause refer to the same entity, then the object will be in the
form of a reflexive pronoun. The reflexive pronoun will agree with the subject
in number, person, and gender, where relevant.
You (S) should see yourself
(DO).
We (S) rewarded ourselves
(DO) with a treat.
All other pronouns assuming object function will take the
objective form.
I like her (DO).
She likes me (DO).
Voice. There is also a
systematic relationship between the object of an active voice clause and the
subject of a passive voice clause. The object of the active voice clause
corresponds to the subject in the passive voice equivalent.
Emily likes Liz (DO). [active voice]
Liz (S) is liked by Emily
[passive voice]
The Indirect Object
The
indirect object is identifiable by all of the criteria of the direct object
with a few unique characteristics of its own in form and position.
Form. Although both objects
usually occur either as noun phrases or as clauses in form, the indirect object
is restricted to the relative clause form.
I sent whoever wants it (IO) copies of a receipt.
Position. The indirect
object can occur only when the direct object is also represented in the clause.
(Here we use the asterisk [*] to represent ungrammatical forms in a language.)
We gave her (IO) everything (DO).
*We gave her (IO).
The
indirect object also occurs only between the verb and the direct object.
We gave (V) her (IO) everything (DO).
*We gave (V) everything (DO) her (IO).
Moreover,
only the indirect object can be paraphrased by a prepositional phrase
functioning as an adverbial, beginning with either to or for. The
choice of the preposition is governed by the main verb of the clause.
We gave her (IO) everything (DO).
We gave everything (DO) to her (A).
Finally,
the indirect object can be omitted without affecting the semantic relationships
of the remaining constituents of the clause.
We (S) gave (V) her
(IO) everything (DO).
We (S) gave (V) everything
(DO).
The Object Complement
The object
complement completes a reference and/or an implication suggested by the object
of the clause. The object complement is identifiable by its form, position,
reference, and agreement.
Form. Object complements are
most usually noun phrases or adjective phrases, although a clause may assume
this function on occasion.
They elected her Chair of the department (OC).
We find this music most pleasant (OC).
Position. Object complements
only occur if there is an object in the clause and then occur normally after
that object.
They elected her Chair of the department (OC).
*They elected Chair of the department (OC).
*They elected Chair of the department (OC) her.
And unlike
objects themselves, object complements do not have any passive voice
corresponding clauses, in which the complement appears in the subject position.
*Chair of the department was elected her by them.
Reference. Since all
complements imply a reference, object complements complete a reference to the
object of the clause. There is in effect a copular relation that exists between
the object and its complement, in that the object and its complement can be
paraphrased by a SVC structure, like this:
They elected her (O) Chair of the department
(C).
She (S) is Chair of the
department (C).
Agreement. Object
complements usually agree with the object in number (singular or plural).
She made Liz and Emily (DO) her assistants
(OC).
She made Frank (DO) her assistant (OC).
The Subject Complement
The
subject complement completes a reference to and/or an implication suggested by
the subject of the clause. It shares many of the properties of the object
complement, notably form and agreement, as described above under the 'Subject'
and 'Object Complement' subheadings. Yet it has a few properties unique to
itself, such as position.
Position. The subject
complement always occurs after a copular verb in the SVC clause pattern. The
most common copular verb in English is the verb be used as a main verb,
although other verbs that relate to perception also serve in this category,
such as appear, seem, look, sound, feel, etc.
The Adverbial and Adverbial Complement
Another
functional constituent that suggests a copular relationship with some other
clause constituent is the adverbial complement. It occurs only in explicit
copular relationships referring to the subject, as in
Liz (S) is in the park
(AC).
or in implicit copular relationships referring to the
object, as in
I put the cookies (DO) in the pantry (AC).
The
adverbial complement referring to the object can be paraphrased in a SVC
clause, as in
The cookies (S) are in the pantry (AC).
Adverbial
complements occur only in those two positions. Adverbials on the other hand are
not usually as restricted in position or in reference.
To understand
more about these last two functions, we should learn to identify adverbials in
general by their form, position, and meaning.
Form. The adverbial is
normally an adverb phrase, prepositional phrase, or a clause. Occasionally, a
noun phrase can function as an adverbial.
Later (A), I will finish the
book.
In the afternoon (A), I will
finish the book.
When I get home (A), I will
finish the book.
Next week (A), I will finish
the book.
Position. As we have seen
above, the adverbial can occur in many different positions with a clause. The
adverbial may be at the beginning (clause-initial), in the middle
(clause-medial), or at the end (clause-final). Indeed, even its clause-medial
position reveals an enormous degree of "flexibility."
Frankly (A), John was
disappointed. [initial]
John, frankly (A), was disappointed. [medial]
John was, frankly (A), disappointed. [medial]
John was disappointed, frankly (A). [Final]
(By contrast, we know that the adverbial complement is
restricted in its position to follow either a copular verb or an object.)
Further we
should note that not all the adverbials in English are as "mobile" as
the example above. Though, generally speaking, adverbials do give the speaker
the greatest degree of positional choice of all the clause constituents.
Meaning. The adverbial
conveys a definable, but wide-ranging, set of meanings.
Yesterday (A), Ted left.
[time]
If possible (A), ring me
later. [contingency]
The book fell on the floor (A). [place]
If it rains (A), we'll leave
later. [condition]
Although he's young (A),
he's good. [concession]
While she slept (A), I
worked. [contrast]
I would go, except I can't (A). [exception]
Knowing her (A), I chose a
red one. [reason]
To open the lock (A), tug on
the door. [purpose]
I fed the stray, to gain its trust (A). [result]
The dog obeyed, as instructed (A). [comparison]
I would fight, rather than quit (A). [preference]
Finally,
we should remember one last, but important, difference between the adverbial
and all other functional constituents: the adverbial is the optional
constituent; it can be left out of the clause; all others are obligatory.
Having
completed this brief survey of the clause, we should always remember, one, that
there is always much more to be said than this meager outline can cover and,
two, that there is always a much greater variety of grammatical structure in
the world's language than this chapter could ever hope to show (cf. Curme 1931
and Quirk et al. 1985).
Nonetheless,
before we end this discussion, we need to talk about a few more points.
Grammatical Ambiguity
Words, as
we know, often have more than one meaning, and that is the classic instance of
ambiguity. Ambiguity has another sense, however. Occasionally, phrases and
clauses create ambiguity because their structures may be interpreted in more
than one way, leading to different meanings for the sentence as a whole. Such
ambiguity is called 'grammatical ambiguity.'
(1) An example of grammatical ambiguity at the phrase
rank, interpreting her duck as a noun phrase
EXAMPLE
|
I
|
saw
|
her
|
duck.
|
FORM
|
Pronoun
|
Verb
|
Determiner
[Noun |
Noun
Phrase] |
FUNCTION
|
Subject
|
Verb
|
Object
|
Compare
tables (1) and (2) for an example of grammatical ambiguity.
(2) An example of grammatical ambiguity at the phrase
rank, interpreting duck as a subordinate clause
EXAMPLE
|
I
|
saw
|
her
|
duck.
|
FORM
|
Pronoun
|
Verb
|
Pronoun
|
Infinitive Verb
|
FUNCTION
|
Subject
|
Verb
|
Direct Object
|
Subordinate Clause
|
At the
clause rank, ambiguity arises when clause constituents can be interpreted as
having two or more functions. Consider the clause They found me a good
worker, where the functions of the last two constituents can vary, creating
two different meanings. Compare tables (3) and (4).
(3) An example of grammatical ambiguity at the clause
rank, interpreting me as an indirect object (meaning "They found a good
worker for me")
EXAMPLE
|
They
|
found
|
me
|
a good worker.
|
FUNCTION
|
Subject
|
Verb
|
Indirect Object
|
Direct Object
|
Now
compare example (4).
(4) An example of grammatical ambiguity at the clause
rank, interpreting me as a direct object (meaning "They feel that I am a
good worker")
EXAMPLE
|
They
|
found
|
me
|
a good worker.
|
FUNCTION
|
Subject
|
Verb
|
Direct Object
|
Object Complement
|
REFERENCES
Curme, George.
1931 A Grammar of the English Language, 2 volumes. New York : D. C. Heath
and Company.
Halliday, Michael A. K.
1994 Introduction to Functional Grammar, 2nd ed. London : Edward Arnold.
Quirk, R., S. Greenbaum, G. Leech, and J. Svartvik.
1985 A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language, London : Longman.
Tidak ada komentar:
Posting Komentar